Coping Responses During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru
Descripción del Articulo
Background. The COVID-19 pandemic has subjected people around the world to severe stress, evoking a variety of coping responses. Coping responses can be broadly classified into four strategies: 1) problem-focused coping; 2) emotion-focused coping; 3) socially supported coping; and 4) avoidance. Whil...
Autores: | , , , , , , , , , , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | artículo |
Fecha de Publicación: | 2020 |
Institución: | Universidad de Lima |
Repositorio: | ULIMA-Institucional |
Lenguaje: | inglés |
OAI Identifier: | oai:repositorio.ulima.edu.pe:20.500.12724/12456 |
Enlace del recurso: | https://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12724/12456 https://doi.org/10.11621/pir.2020.0404 |
Nivel de acceso: | acceso abierto |
Materia: | Human behavior Cross-cultural studies COVID-19 Conducta humana Estudios interculturales https://purl.org/pe-repo/ocde/ford#2.02.04 |
id |
RULI_08106ac43264e47064318c8d6ce812b9 |
---|---|
oai_identifier_str |
oai:repositorio.ulima.edu.pe:20.500.12724/12456 |
network_acronym_str |
RULI |
network_name_str |
ULIMA-Institucional |
repository_id_str |
3883 |
dc.title.en_EN.fl_str_mv |
Coping Responses During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru |
title |
Coping Responses During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru |
spellingShingle |
Coping Responses During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru Voronin, Ivan A. Human behavior Cross-cultural studies COVID-19 Conducta humana Estudios interculturales https://purl.org/pe-repo/ocde/ford#2.02.04 |
title_short |
Coping Responses During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru |
title_full |
Coping Responses During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru |
title_fullStr |
Coping Responses During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru |
title_full_unstemmed |
Coping Responses During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru |
title_sort |
Coping Responses During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru |
author |
Voronin, Ivan A. |
author_facet |
Voronin, Ivan A. Manrique Millones, Denisse Lisette Vasin, Georgy M. Millones Rivalles, Rosa Bertha Manrique Pino, Oscar Fernandez Rios, Nataly Marakshina, Yulia A. Lobaskova, M. M. Symanyuk, Elvira E. Pecherkina, Anna A. Ageeva, Irina A. . Lysenkova, Irina A. Ismatullina, Victoria I. Sitnikova, Maria A. Malykh, Sergey B. |
author_role |
author |
author2 |
Manrique Millones, Denisse Lisette Vasin, Georgy M. Millones Rivalles, Rosa Bertha Manrique Pino, Oscar Fernandez Rios, Nataly Marakshina, Yulia A. Lobaskova, M. M. Symanyuk, Elvira E. Pecherkina, Anna A. Ageeva, Irina A. . Lysenkova, Irina A. Ismatullina, Victoria I. Sitnikova, Maria A. Malykh, Sergey B. |
author2_role |
author author author author author author author author author author author author author author |
dc.contributor.other.none.fl_str_mv |
Millones Rivalles, Rosa Bertha |
dc.contributor.author.fl_str_mv |
Voronin, Ivan A. Manrique Millones, Denisse Lisette Vasin, Georgy M. Millones Rivalles, Rosa Bertha Manrique Pino, Oscar Fernandez Rios, Nataly Marakshina, Yulia A. Lobaskova, M. M. Symanyuk, Elvira E. Pecherkina, Anna A. Ageeva, Irina A. . Lysenkova, Irina A. Ismatullina, Victoria I. Sitnikova, Maria A. Malykh, Sergey B. |
dc.subject.en_EN.fl_str_mv |
Human behavior Cross-cultural studies |
topic |
Human behavior Cross-cultural studies COVID-19 Conducta humana Estudios interculturales https://purl.org/pe-repo/ocde/ford#2.02.04 |
dc.subject.es_PE.fl_str_mv |
COVID-19 Conducta humana Estudios interculturales |
dc.subject.ocde.none.fl_str_mv |
https://purl.org/pe-repo/ocde/ford#2.02.04 |
description |
Background. The COVID-19 pandemic has subjected people around the world to severe stress, evoking a variety of coping responses. Coping responses can be broadly classified into four strategies: 1) problem-focused coping; 2) emotion-focused coping; 3) socially supported coping; and 4) avoidance. While there is a wide variability of individual coping responses, to some extent they are also culturally specific. Objective. This study aimed to compare the differences in the prevalence and factor structure of coping responses during COVID-19 pandemic in three countries: Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru. Design. The sample included 501 participants from Russia, 456 participants from Kyrgyzstan, and 354 participants from Peru. The mean age of participants was 28 years in Russia (SD = 13.5); 24 years in Kyrgyzstan (SD = 10.0); and 30 years in Peru (SD = 12.3). In Russia and Kyrgyzstan, coping strategies were assessed with an abbreviated Russian adaptation of the COPE (Coping Orientations to Problems Experienced) questionnaire. In Peru, coping responses were assessed using the Spanish version of the Brief COPE questionnaire. The average scores from fifteen COPE scales were used as the input data for linear modelling and factor analysis. Results. The coping scores varied substantially within each country. Differences between countries accounted for 17.7% of the total variability in religious coping; 15.8% in acceptance; 13.9% in mental disengagement; and less than 7% in the other coping strategies. No difference in the prevalence of coping responses was found between Russian and Kyrgyz participants after accounting for age and gender. In all three countries the coping responses were associated with the same four coping domains: problem-focused coping, socially supported coping, avoidance, and emotion-focused coping. Four factors explained up to 44% of the total variation in the COPE scores. Religious coping and mental disengagement were classified into different coping domains in the three countries. Conclusion. The results suggest that during the COVID-19 pandemic, people from different countries apply the full range of coping responses within the four universal coping strategies. Religious coping and mental disengagement differed the most across the countries, suggesting that some coping behaviors can take on different roles within the system of coping responses to stressful events. We attribute these differences to differing cultural and socioeconomic characteristics, and the different measures taken by governments in response to COVID-19. |
publishDate |
2020 |
dc.date.accessioned.none.fl_str_mv |
2021-02-11T16:46:23Z |
dc.date.available.none.fl_str_mv |
2021-02-11T16:46:23Z |
dc.date.issued.fl_str_mv |
2020 |
dc.type.none.fl_str_mv |
info:eu-repo/semantics/article |
dc.type.other.none.fl_str_mv |
Artículo en Scopus |
format |
article |
dc.identifier.citation.es_PE.fl_str_mv |
Voronin, I.A., Manrique-Millones, D., Vasin, G.M., Millones-Rivalles, R.B., Manrique-Pino, O., Fernandez-Rios, N., Marakshina, Y.A., Lobaskova, M.M., Symanyuk, E.E., Pecherkina, A.A., Ageeva, I.A., Lysenkova, I.A., Ismatullina, V.I., Sitnikova, M.A. & Malykh, S.B. (2020). Coping Responses During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 13(4), 55-74. https://doi.org/10.11621/pir.2020.0404 |
dc.identifier.issn.none.fl_str_mv |
2074-6857 |
dc.identifier.uri.none.fl_str_mv |
https://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12724/12456 |
dc.identifier.journal.none.fl_str_mv |
Psychology in Russia: State of the Art |
dc.identifier.isni.none.fl_str_mv |
0000000121541816 |
dc.identifier.doi.none.fl_str_mv |
https://doi.org/10.11621/pir.2020.0404 |
dc.identifier.scopusid.none.fl_str_mv |
2-s2.0-85100034392 |
identifier_str_mv |
Voronin, I.A., Manrique-Millones, D., Vasin, G.M., Millones-Rivalles, R.B., Manrique-Pino, O., Fernandez-Rios, N., Marakshina, Y.A., Lobaskova, M.M., Symanyuk, E.E., Pecherkina, A.A., Ageeva, I.A., Lysenkova, I.A., Ismatullina, V.I., Sitnikova, M.A. & Malykh, S.B. (2020). Coping Responses During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 13(4), 55-74. https://doi.org/10.11621/pir.2020.0404 2074-6857 Psychology in Russia: State of the Art 0000000121541816 2-s2.0-85100034392 |
url |
https://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12724/12456 https://doi.org/10.11621/pir.2020.0404 |
dc.language.iso.none.fl_str_mv |
eng |
language |
eng |
dc.relation.ispartof.none.fl_str_mv |
urn:issn:2074-6857 |
dc.rights.*.fl_str_mv |
info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess |
dc.rights.uri.*.fl_str_mv |
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/ |
eu_rights_str_mv |
openAccess |
rights_invalid_str_mv |
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/ |
dc.format.none.fl_str_mv |
application/html |
dc.publisher.none.fl_str_mv |
Russsian Psychological Society |
dc.publisher.country.none.fl_str_mv |
RU |
publisher.none.fl_str_mv |
Russsian Psychological Society |
dc.source.none.fl_str_mv |
Repositorio Institucional - Ulima Universidad de Lima reponame:ULIMA-Institucional instname:Universidad de Lima instacron:ULIMA |
instname_str |
Universidad de Lima |
instacron_str |
ULIMA |
institution |
ULIMA |
reponame_str |
ULIMA-Institucional |
collection |
ULIMA-Institucional |
bitstream.url.fl_str_mv |
https://repositorio.ulima.edu.pe/bitstream/20.500.12724/12456/2/license_rdf https://repositorio.ulima.edu.pe/bitstream/20.500.12724/12456/3/license.txt |
bitstream.checksum.fl_str_mv |
8fc46f5e71650fd7adee84a69b9163c2 8a4605be74aa9ea9d79846c1fba20a33 |
bitstream.checksumAlgorithm.fl_str_mv |
MD5 MD5 |
repository.name.fl_str_mv |
Repositorio Universidad de Lima |
repository.mail.fl_str_mv |
repositorio@ulima.edu.pe |
_version_ |
1845977361453940736 |
spelling |
Voronin, Ivan A.Manrique Millones, Denisse LisetteVasin, Georgy M.Millones Rivalles, Rosa BerthaManrique Pino, OscarFernandez Rios, NatalyMarakshina, Yulia A.Lobaskova, M. M.Symanyuk, Elvira E.Pecherkina, Anna A.Ageeva, Irina A. .Lysenkova, Irina A.Ismatullina, Victoria I.Sitnikova, Maria A.Malykh, Sergey B.Millones Rivalles, Rosa Bertha2021-02-11T16:46:23Z2021-02-11T16:46:23Z2020Voronin, I.A., Manrique-Millones, D., Vasin, G.M., Millones-Rivalles, R.B., Manrique-Pino, O., Fernandez-Rios, N., Marakshina, Y.A., Lobaskova, M.M., Symanyuk, E.E., Pecherkina, A.A., Ageeva, I.A., Lysenkova, I.A., Ismatullina, V.I., Sitnikova, M.A. & Malykh, S.B. (2020). Coping Responses During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 13(4), 55-74. https://doi.org/10.11621/pir.2020.04042074-6857https://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12724/12456Psychology in Russia: State of the Art0000000121541816https://doi.org/10.11621/pir.2020.04042-s2.0-85100034392Background. The COVID-19 pandemic has subjected people around the world to severe stress, evoking a variety of coping responses. Coping responses can be broadly classified into four strategies: 1) problem-focused coping; 2) emotion-focused coping; 3) socially supported coping; and 4) avoidance. While there is a wide variability of individual coping responses, to some extent they are also culturally specific. Objective. This study aimed to compare the differences in the prevalence and factor structure of coping responses during COVID-19 pandemic in three countries: Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peru. Design. The sample included 501 participants from Russia, 456 participants from Kyrgyzstan, and 354 participants from Peru. The mean age of participants was 28 years in Russia (SD = 13.5); 24 years in Kyrgyzstan (SD = 10.0); and 30 years in Peru (SD = 12.3). In Russia and Kyrgyzstan, coping strategies were assessed with an abbreviated Russian adaptation of the COPE (Coping Orientations to Problems Experienced) questionnaire. In Peru, coping responses were assessed using the Spanish version of the Brief COPE questionnaire. The average scores from fifteen COPE scales were used as the input data for linear modelling and factor analysis. Results. The coping scores varied substantially within each country. Differences between countries accounted for 17.7% of the total variability in religious coping; 15.8% in acceptance; 13.9% in mental disengagement; and less than 7% in the other coping strategies. No difference in the prevalence of coping responses was found between Russian and Kyrgyz participants after accounting for age and gender. In all three countries the coping responses were associated with the same four coping domains: problem-focused coping, socially supported coping, avoidance, and emotion-focused coping. Four factors explained up to 44% of the total variation in the COPE scores. Religious coping and mental disengagement were classified into different coping domains in the three countries. Conclusion. The results suggest that during the COVID-19 pandemic, people from different countries apply the full range of coping responses within the four universal coping strategies. Religious coping and mental disengagement differed the most across the countries, suggesting that some coping behaviors can take on different roles within the system of coping responses to stressful events. We attribute these differences to differing cultural and socioeconomic characteristics, and the different measures taken by governments in response to COVID-19.application/htmlengRusssian Psychological SocietyRUurn:issn:2074-6857info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccesshttps://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/Repositorio Institucional - UlimaUniversidad de Limareponame:ULIMA-Institucionalinstname:Universidad de Limainstacron:ULIMAHuman behaviorCross-cultural studiesCOVID-19Conducta humanaEstudios interculturaleshttps://purl.org/pe-repo/ocde/ford#2.02.04Coping Responses During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Russia, Kyrgyzstan, and Peruinfo:eu-repo/semantics/articleArtículo en ScopusNo figura en la lista del año 2020Universidad de LimaOICC-LICENSElicense_rdflicense_rdfapplication/rdf+xml; charset=utf-81037https://repositorio.ulima.edu.pe/bitstream/20.500.12724/12456/2/license_rdf8fc46f5e71650fd7adee84a69b9163c2MD52LICENSElicense.txtlicense.txttext/plain; charset=utf-81748https://repositorio.ulima.edu.pe/bitstream/20.500.12724/12456/3/license.txt8a4605be74aa9ea9d79846c1fba20a33MD5320.500.12724/12456oai:repositorio.ulima.edu.pe:20.500.12724/124562025-03-06 19:32:08.902Repositorio Universidad de Limarepositorio@ulima.edu.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 |
score |
13.024418 |
Nota importante:
La información contenida en este registro es de entera responsabilidad de la institución que gestiona el repositorio institucional donde esta contenido este documento o set de datos. El CONCYTEC no se hace responsable por los contenidos (publicaciones y/o datos) accesibles a través del Repositorio Nacional Digital de Ciencia, Tecnología e Innovación de Acceso Abierto (ALICIA).
La información contenida en este registro es de entera responsabilidad de la institución que gestiona el repositorio institucional donde esta contenido este documento o set de datos. El CONCYTEC no se hace responsable por los contenidos (publicaciones y/o datos) accesibles a través del Repositorio Nacional Digital de Ciencia, Tecnología e Innovación de Acceso Abierto (ALICIA).